◯ Goodbye to dental fillings? ‘No-drill’ techniques.虫歯を削る必要はないと判明WashPost

2016-01-20 20:57:51 | ♪PFK ASAP NEWS
2016-01-20 20:57:51 washingtonpost

https://www.washingtonpost.com
/news/to-your-health/wp/2015/12/08/goodbye-to-dental-fillings-no-drill-techniques-can-treat-tooth-decay-just-as-well-study-finds/

To Your Health
Goodbye to dental fillings?

‘No-drill’ techniques can treat tooth decay just as well, study finds


Resize Text Print Article Comments 23

By Ariana Eunjung Cha December 8, 2015

(iStock)
If you've ever endured a dental filling based on the advice that there's no other way to stop tooth decay before it gets worse, you may be a bit, well, annoyed when you hear about the latest study on this topic.

In a seven-year study involving 1,000 patients at 22 dental practices, researchers at the University of Sydney compared people who received traditional "drill and fill" treatment with those who received a less aggressive preventive treatment that involves high concentration fluoride varnish and restricting sugary snacks and beverages between meals.

They found that for those in the second group the need for fillings declined by 30 to 50 percent. Among those at highest risk, who were getting as many as two fillings per year, the reduction was even greater -- 80 percent.

The conclusion, according to lead author Wendell Evans, is that much decay can be stopped, reversed, and prevented with simple treatments: "It's unnecessary for patients to have fillings because they're not required in many cases of dental decay."

The results, published in Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology this week, signals that a major shift may be needed in how dentists treat early tooth decay, the researchers said.

Current dental practices are based on the assumption that tooth decay is rapidly progressive -- and possibly inevitable. If that were the case it would make sense to identify decay early and remove it by drilling and filling. But newer studies show that decay develops more slowly, an average of four to eight years to progress from the other layer of the tooth's enamel to the inner layer where it can do the most damage.

"That is plenty of time," the researchers said, "for the decay to be detected and treated before it becomes a cavity and requires a filling."

Unfortunately, if the decay has already formed into a cavity, there's really no way to avoid the filling.

Read more:

Chipotle CEO pledges to make restaurants ‘safest place to eat.’ Here’s his plan.

Netflix binge-watchers beware: Watching too much TV in your 20s may impact how your brain works in mid-life, study suggests

With $45 billion pledge to charity, Mark Zuckerberg and wife imagine ‘a world without suffering from disease’

The myth of sugar-free drinks, candy: Study shows they can wreak havoc on teeth, too

Modern men tend to overeat like cavemen as a way of showing off to women

For more health news, you can sign up for our weekly newsletter here.




Ariana Eunjung Cha is a national reporter. She has previously served as the Post's bureau chief in Shanghai and San Francisco, and as a correspondent in Baghdad.






2016-01-20 20:57:51 washingtonpost

https://www.washingtonpost.com
/news/to-your-health/wp/2015/12/08/goodbye-to-dental-fillings-no-drill-techniques-can-treat-tooth-decay-just-as-well-study-finds/
2016年01月20日 06時00分00秒
虫歯を削る必要はないと判明、

虫歯治療の根本概念が変わる可能性

By Finizio

一般的な虫歯の治療法と言えば、「虫歯を削って詰め物をする」というものですが、最新の研究で「虫歯を削る必要なし」「詰め物も不要」という実験結果が現れて、虫歯治療が根本的に変わる可能性が指摘されています。

“No-drill” dentistry stops tooth decay - new research - The University of Sydney
http://sydney.edu.au/news-opinion/news/2015/12/07/_no-drill_-dentistry-stops-tooth-decay---new-research.html

Goodbye to dental fillings? ‘No-drill’ techniques can treat tooth decay just as well, study finds - The Washington Post
https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/to-your-health/wp/2015/12/08/goodbye-to-dental-fillings-no-drill-techniques-can-treat-tooth-decay-just-as-well-study-finds/

シドニー大学のウェンデル・エバンス准教授らの研究チームは、22の歯科医療機関と連携してのべ1000人の患者を調査する7年間にもわたる実験の結果から、「ほとんどの虫歯治療にはドリルで削ったり、詰め物したりする必要はない」との結論を出しました。

実験では、虫歯患者を従来通りのドリルで削って詰め物をするグループと、ドリルで削らずに高濃度のフッ素塗布をした上で、砂糖の入ったお菓子や飲み物の間食を制限する「オーラルケア」と呼ばれる治療法を採るグループとに分けて、虫歯の経過を観察しました。

その結果、ドリルで削らなかったオーラルケア・グループの虫歯は30%から50%が減少し、中でも1年間に2本の虫歯治療が必要とされる虫歯リスクの高い層に限って言えば、虫歯の減少率は80%も高かったことが分かりました。この結果を受けて、エバンス准教授は、「ほとんどの虫歯のケースでドリルで削って詰め物をする必要はない」と述べています。

By get directly down

これまで歯科学会では「虫歯の進行速度は極めて速く、この進行速度を遅くしたり、反対に虫歯を健康な状態に回復させたりすることは不可能である」という固定概念のような定説が支配的であり、そのため、早期に虫歯を削り取るのが良いとされてきたとのこと。しかし、エバンス准教授の実験結果からは、虫歯を削るよりもオーラルケアの方が虫歯の治療に有効であるという、定説を覆す結論が得られそうです。

なお、大きな穴が空いたような虫歯の場合はもはや手遅れで、ドリルで削り取り詰め物をする従来通りの治療しか有効な手立てはないとのこと。しかし、虫歯が発生し始めてから、一定の時間内の比較的小さな状態であれば、ドリルで削ることなくオーラルケアで治療できるそうです。

エバンス准教授によると50年来続くオーラルケア治療の研究からは、虫歯の進行速度はいつも急激であるとは限らず、それまで考えられてきたよりもはるかにゆっくりであることが分かったとのこと。例えば、虫歯は、歯の外側のエナメル層から内側の象牙質層に"侵攻"するのに4年から8年かかり、その間であれば虫歯を発見してドリルで削る治療をすることなく治療できるそうです。

By Partha S. Sahana

エバンス准教授は正しいオーラルケアとしてCaries Management System (CMS)という方法を提唱しています。これによると、虫歯を削ることなくオーラルケアする条件として「初期段階の虫歯に高濃度のフッ素塗布治療を施すこと」「正しい歯磨き方法を身につけること」「砂糖の入ったスナック菓子や飲み物の間食を控えること」「虫歯の経過を観察し続けること」を挙げており、削って詰め物をして終わりという1回キリの治療ではなく、歯科医師と協力した継続的な治療が必要だそうです。

◯ The Work We Do While We Sleep / 人は30年分眠ってる

2016-01-20 07:44:11 | ♪ One Short Talk
2016-01-20 07:44:11 The New Yorker. gigazine net


Maria Konnikova
MARIA KONNIKOVA

JULY 8, 2015
The Work We Do While We Sleep

BY MARIA KONNIKOVA
part series on sleep. Read part one, on falling asleep, and part three, on wakefulness.

It’s strange, when you think about it, that we spend close to a third of our lives asleep. Why do we do it? While we’re sleeping, we’re vulnerable―and, at least on the outside, supremely unproductive. In a 1719 sermon, “Vigilius, or, The Awakener,” Cotton Mather called an excess of sleep “sinful” and lamented that we often sleep when we should be working. Benjamin Franklin echoed the sentiment in “Poor Richard’s Almanack,” when he quipped that “there’ll be sleeping enough in the grave.” For a long time, sleep’s apparent uselessness amused even the scientists who studied it. The Harvard sleep researcher Robert Stickgold has recalled his former collaborator J. Allan Hobson joking that the only known function of sleep was to cure sleepiness. In a 2006 review of the explanations researchers had proposed for sleep, Marcos Frank, a neuroscientist then working at the University of Pennsylvania (he is now at WSU Spokane) concluded that the evidence for sleep’s putative effects on cognition was “weak or equivocal.”


But in the past decade, and even the past year, the mystery has seemed to be abating. In a series of conversations with sleep scientists this May, I was offered a glimpse of converging lines of inquiry that are shedding light on why such a significant part of our lives is spent lying inert, with our eyes closed, not doing anything that seems particularly meaningful or relevant to, well, anything. (The meetings were facilitated by a Harvard Medical School Media Fellowship.)

One line of evidence stems from what happens when sleep is disturbed. By looking at what can go wrong while we’re asleep, we can get a sense of what sleep is for. Take genetic impairments. In REM-sleep behavior disorder, the usual paralysis that keeps our bodies in check as we enter REM sleep disappears, and people physically act out their dreams. The disorder is a fairly reliable predictor of neurodegenerative disease: one study, based at the Center for Sleep Research at the Hôpital du Sacré-Coeur, in Montreal, found that over half of the patients who acted out their dreams had developed some form of neurodegenerative disease twelve years later. There’s sleep apnea, when your breathing pauses as you sleep, often for a few seconds but sometimes for a few minutes, before your body briefly jolts awake to restart the process. (Often, the jolt never makes it into conscious memory.) In trials, the disorder has been associated with diabetes and cardiovascular disease and has been shown to lead to cognitive impairment. About ten per cent of the population experience impairment during their waking hours as a result of chronic insomnia, a condition that has been linked to low quality of life, depression, increases in cardiovascular disease and hypertension, cognitive and motor impairment, and a number of other medical conditions.

For sleep scientists, all of these unpleasant disorders offer tantalizing lines of inquiry. The link between many of them and neurodegenerative disease, or other forms of cognitive impairment, suggests that sleep could be required for cognitive upkeep and function. Their link with heart disease suggests that sleep might serve to relieve vascular stress. The fact that insomnia is associated with depression suggests that sleep might help us deal with emotionally stressful or otherwise disruptive events.

Increasingly, researchers have found ways to test these theories more directly. In 2000, Stickgold published a study in Science that became one of the most convincing validations of the role that sleep and dreams play in memory consolidation. For seven hours, over the course of three days, a group of people played the computer game Tetris. Some had never played the game before; others were familiar with it; and a third group didn’t know either way, because they were amnesiacs with extensive damage to their medial temporal lobes and hippocampi. Their particular form of amnesia meant that they were unable to form new episodic memories.

Each night, as they were falling asleep, Stickgold’s subjects were repeatedly awoken and asked to recall, to the best of their knowledge, what they were dreaming about. It turned out that they dreamed of Tetris. What was surprising was that even the amnesiacs had these dreams. They had no idea what they were seeing and, the next morning they remembered neither the game nor the experimenter, but they would recall dreams of falling shapes that conformed to the Tetris patterns they’d observed earlier in the day. Sometimes, they’d even report that the shapes rotated to fit into rows.

Since then, evidence for the memory-related functions of sleep, and of dreams in particular, has only mounted. In 2013, Stickgold published a summary of his research since the Tetris study, also reviewing parallel advances by other researchers in the field. In that paper, he argued that sleep isn’t crucial just for memory consolidation; it is also a remarkably selective mechanism. We don’t remember everything that happens to us on a given day: sometimes, we remember something simply because it’s emotional, while, at other times, we work our way through mundane details to figure out why something matters. Sleeping and dreaming, Stickgold argues, help us sift through material to isolate and store the important take-away, whether it is what he calls the “gist” (the overarching point of a lot of information) or a specific detail. “When we dream, we get the pieces. When we wake, we can know the whole,” Stickgold says.


In one experiment, the University of Tübingen neurobiologist Jan Born and Ullrich Wagner, a neuroscientist at the University of Münster, taught a group of people a relatively complex math task. Though the subjects didn’t know it, there was a simpler way of solving the problem―an abstract rule that would enable a quick solution. Few of the subjects spontaneously figured out the solution the first time. Each participant was retested on the task eight hours later; some were allowed to sleep and others had to remain awake. Just under a quarter of the group that took a sleepless break came up with the faster solution. But the insight rate more than doubled among the subjects who had spent the eight hours sleeping: sixty per cent of them could now see the shortcut. As we sleep, our brains replay, process, learn, and extract meaning. In a sense, they think.

Our physical health, too, seems to be intimately tied with sleep. In one study, designed to test the impact that cardiac function has on sleep, and vice versa, a group of physiologists subjected healthy men with no history of cardiac problems to sleep deprivation. At various points, they measured their vascular fitness, checking their heart rates, their blood pressure, and their levels of certain proteins associated with heart problems. Within two days, almost every marker was elevated. Conversely, in a study looking at sleep apnea, which is strongly linked to cardiovascular disease, treatment of the apnea was found to improve vascular function in short order: once patients could sleep soundly, the stress on their hearts was relieved.

But the importance of sleep to our brain function may be even more fundamental. In addition to its memory and problem-solving functions, sleep may help our brains stay sharp, young, and healthy. Two years ago, the University of Rochester neuroscientist Maiken Nedergaard published the results of many years of research into the function of sleep. After using new techniques to peer into the waking and sleeping brains of mice, she discovered that sleep was the brain’s maintenance system. When we’re awake, our activities lead to a build-up of debris in the brain: we form toxins, such as beta-amyloid, a protein associated with Alzheimer’s, and other proteins that are usually harmless get misfolded. When we fall asleep, specific channels in our brains expand to allow cerebrospinal fluid to flow in and clear that debris. These mechanisms have been termed the “glymphatic system,” a nod to the lymphatic system, which clears waste from our bodies Conversely, when our brains don’t have enough time to rest, toxins build up, and neurodegenerative disease sets in. Indeed, one of the earliest signs of impending dementia is sleep disturbance, and some of the genes that control sleep duration are also implicated in schizophrenia. Sleep disruption may share a common biological mechanism with neurodegenerative diseases.

All of this research suggests that important work happens while we sleep. And yet we also know that few of us sleep enough. Some of us may no longer even know what being fully awake feels like. How is your mind today affected by a lack of sleep the night before? Increasingly, researchers are turning their attention to waking life, and asking how much it’s been changed by chronic lack of sleep.


2016-01-20 07:44:11 The New Yorker. gigazine net

2016年01月18日 23時00分00秒
なぜ人間は人生の3分の1もの時間を睡眠に費やす必要があるのか?

by Tony Alter

1日の平均睡眠時間が8時間だとすると、人間は人生の3分の1を睡眠に費やすことになります。これは、90歳まで生きる人であれば、人生の30年分は眠っているという計算です。なぜ人はこんなにも眠りを必要とするのでしょうか。

The Work We Do While We Sleep - The New Yorker
http://www.newyorker.com/science/maria-konnikova/why-we-sleep


The New Yorkerのマリア・コニコヴァさんの記事によると、18世紀には睡眠は罪深いことであるとされていたそうで、また、長らく眠りを研究する学者の間でも、明らかに無益なものだとみられていた時期があるそうです。アメリカの精神医学者で睡眠の研究をしていたアラン・ホブソン氏は「眠りの効果は、眠気を覚ましてくれることだけだ」というジョークを持ちネタにしていたほど。しかし、研究が進むに連れて、謎はだんだんと明かされてきています。

レム睡眠行動障害では、人々は夢見た内容をそのまま実体験しているかのように体が動き出してしまいます。モントリオールにある研究センターによると、この行動障害が起きた人のうち半数以上で、12年以内に神経疾患が見られたとのこと。また、睡眠時無呼吸症候群は糖尿病・心疾患との関係があり、認識機能障害に結びつくことが示されています。さらに、慢性不眠症の人の10%はうつ病、心疾患と高血圧の増加、認識力・運動不全などに悩まされているそうです。

睡眠科学者によると、こうした神経疾患との関連は睡眠が認識機能に影響を与えていることを、心疾患との関連は睡眠が血管へのストレスを和らげる役割を果たしていることを、不眠症とうつとの関連は睡眠が情緒面へも影響を与えていることを、それぞれ示唆しているとのこと。

2000年に、ハーバード大学で睡眠を研究しているロバート・スティックゴールド氏は、3つのグループに対して1日7時間、合計3日間にわたってテトリスをプレイしてもらう実験を行いました。グループ1はテトリスをしたことがない人たち、グループ2はテトリスに慣れた人たち。そして、グループ3は側頭葉や海馬にダメージを受けた健忘症患者で、新たなエピソード記憶を形成することができません。しかし、実験期間中にどんな夢を見たかを確認したところ、グループ1、グループ2だけではなく、グループ3の人すらテトリスの夢を見ていたことがわかりました。彼らは「テトリスが何か」もわからない状態なので、「自分たちが何の夢を見ていたのか」もわかっていない状態でしたが、その話に出てきた物体の形状はテトリミノの形と一致しており、時には、テトリミノが隙間に入るために回転するところまで夢に出てきたと証言しました。

このように、睡眠には記憶関連、特に夢に関する機能もあることが浮上。

by Umberto Salvagnin

さらに、テュービンゲン大学の神経生物学者ジャン・ボルン氏、ウルリッヒ・ヴァーグナー氏は、睡眠が「記憶の統一」だけではなく「記憶の選択」のメカニズムも備えていることを明らかにしました。ボルン氏らは実験で、一群の人々に複雑な数学の問題を出しました。実は、簡単な解法が示されているのですが、多くの人が気付かず、問題を解くことができませんでした。参加者は8時間後に再テストを受けましたが、グループのうち半数は眠りを挟んでの挑戦、半数は眠らずに挑戦しました。すると、不眠グループの正解率は25%を切ったのに対して、睡眠グループはその倍以上が正解。また、60%以上の人が簡単な解法に気付きました。

このことから、睡眠は脳内情報の処理・学習・抽出などをしているとも考えられます。睡眠中、いろいろな機能をこなしていると考えると、1日に数時間の睡眠というのは妥当な数字なのかもしれません。

痴呆の初期症状として睡眠障害があることが知られており、また、睡眠時間をコントロールする遺伝子のうちいくつかは統合失調症との結びつきが判明しています。かつての認識とは正反対に、睡眠は体に有益なものであり、むしろ睡眠不足こそ体に悪いものなので、くれぐれも睡眠は大事にしてください。



2016年01月18日 23時00分00秒 in サイエンス, Posted by logc_nt